1
Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075, Vol. 72: e54616, enero-diciembre 2024 (Publicado Ene. 23, 2024)
The role of neophobia in a pioneer olfactory enrichment
for amazons, macaws, and toucans
Mª Carmen Hernández1,2*; https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6918-6761
Isabel Barja1,3; https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8354-5342
1. Unidad Zoología, Departamento de Biología, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, C/ Darwin 2, Campus Universitario
de Cantoblanco, 28049, Madrid, Spain; carmen.hdez.glez@gmail.com (*Correspondence)
2. Instituto de Investigación en Recursos Cinegéticos (IREC), CSIC-UCLM-JCCM, Ronda de Toledo 12, 13005, Ciudad
Real, Spain.
3. Centro de Investigación en Biodiversidad y Cambio Global (CIBC-UAM) Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, C.
Darwin 2, E-28049, Madrid, Spain; isabel.barja@uam.es
Received 17-IV-2023. Corrected 10-X-2023. Accepted 16-I-2024.
ABSTRACT
Introduction: Despite sensory enrichment being critical for ensuring the well–being of captive wild animals,
smells are not being included in enrichment protocols for birds. For this group, neophobia can be a problem
when it comes to implementing new enrichment devices.
Objective: To explore how participation in an olfactory enrichment and latency times varies between bird taxo-
nomic groups (Amazona spp. / Ara spp. / Ramphastos spp.).
Methods: We exposed 257 birds to a scent enrichment, and we recorded which individuals engaged with it and
the time they took to interact with it.
Results: We discovered that participation by toucans in the enrichment was higher compared to amazons and
macaws. Furthermore, latency time to interact with the enrichment was higher in amazons that in the other spe-
cies. Our findings could suggest that toucans are neophilic species which could benefit from higher exploration
rates. Amazons on the contrary seem to be particularly neophobic, possibly because of their less opportunistic
feeding habits compared to toucans and their higher vulnerability to predation compared to macaws.
Conclusion: These results point out that toucans would be more inclined to engage in environmental enrich-
ments, while a more natural design using smells inside familiar objects could be a more successful enrichment
for psittacids.
Key words: psittacine; ramphastids; captivity; animal welfare; rescue center; Costa Rica.
RESUMEN
El papel de la neofobia en un enriquecimiento olfativo pionero para guacamayos, loros y tucanes
Introducción: A pesar de que el enriquecimiento sensorial es fundamental para garantizar el bienestar de los ani-
males silvestres en cautiverio, los olores no son incluidos de forma rutinaria en los protocolos de enriquecimiento
para aves. Además, en el caso de estos animales, la neofobia puede ser un problema a la hora de implementar
nuevos dispositivos de enriquecimiento.
Objetivo: Explorar cómo varía la participación y la latencia en la interacción con un enriquecimiento olfativo
entre grupos taxonómicos de aves (Amazona spp. / Ara spp. / Ramphastos spp.).
Métodos: Expusimos a 257 aves a un enriquecimiento olfativo y registramos qué individuos participaron y el
tiempo que tardaron en interactuar con él.
https://doi.org/10.15517/rev.biol.trop..v72i1.54616
VERTEBRATE BIOLOGY
2Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075 Vol. 72: e54616, enero-diciembre 2024 (Publicado Ene. 23, 2024)
INTRODUCTION
Environmental enrichment can be defined
as any modification in the environment of
captive animals which improves their biologi-
cal functioning (Newberry, 1995). Enrichment
provides the opportunity to display a wider
range of adaptive behaviours and reduces the
frequency of abnormal ones, enhancing animal
welfare (Newberry, 1995; Young, 2013). There
exist a diverse variety of enrichment types:
nutritional, sensory, cognitive, social, physi-
cal features in their environment and so forth
(Newberry, 1995; Young, 2013). Particularly,
olfactory enrichment has been extensively used
in mammals (Wells, 2009), especially for felids
(Clark & King, 2008), but the importance of
the olfactory dimension for other taxa such
as reptiles and birds has been systematically
overlooked (Nielsen et al., 2015). Birds kept in
captivity often develop maladaptive conducts
such as spot picking, route tracing and feather
damaging behaviour (i.e. repetitive self-groom-
ing including picking, plucking or chewing on
feathers) (Brilot et al., 2009; Colton & Fraley,
2014; de Haas & van der Eijk, 2018; Jenkins,
2001; Mellor et al., 2018; Samson, 1996; van
Hoek & Ten-Cate, 1998), this last one being
particularly common in captive Psittaciformes
(Jenkins, 2001; Mellor et al., 2021). Psitta-
cine intellectual capacity appears to be similar
to great apes and marine mammals (Kalmar
et al., 2007; Krasheninnikova et al., 2019),
hence, captive Psittaciformes require high levels
of cognitive stimulation to prevent abnormal
behaviours (Kalmar et al., 2010; Mellor et al.,
2021; Rodriguez-Lopez, 2016). Although the
benefits of implementing enrichment for par-
rots has been extensively described in the litera-
ture (Kalmar et al., 2007; Lumeij & Hommers,
2008; Rodriguez-Lopez, 2016; Young, 2013), it
seems like routine enrichment for psittacines
is constrained to only two types: foraging and
physical modifications of their environment,
whereas sensory-based enrichment has been
neglected (Rodriguez-Lopez, 2016).
In the case of Ramphastids, there is a
concerning lack of research regarding which
types of enrichment they would benefit from,
with only one study evaluating the effects of
conspecific playbacks on a pair of Toco toucans
Ramphastos toco (O’Brien, 2006). Psittacines
and toucans are among the most common bird
groups kept in rescue and rehabilitation cen-
tres across Central America (Hernández pers.
obsv.). Thus, developing and implementing
new enrichments to ensure high quality welfare
standards must be a priority.
One challenge when it comes to design-
ing new enrichment techniques is neophobia
(Clark & King, 2008). Neophobia is described
as the innate aversion to novel stimuli (Green-
berg, 1990; Greenberg & Mettke-Hofmann,
2001). It is considered as an adaptive response
because engaging an unknown object, food or
location may entail being harmed or even killed
by a predator, competitor, or poisonous food
(Greenberg, 1990; Greenberg, 2003; Greenberg
& Mettke-Hofmann, 2001). However, neopho-
bia also carries disadvantages, as it prevents
individuals from interacting with potentially
new fitness-enhancing resources, obtaining less
Resultados: La participación en el enriquecimiento fue mayor en los tucanes en comparación con las amazonas
y los guacamayos. Además, el tiempo de latencia para interactuar con el enriquecimiento fue mayor en las ama-
zonas que en las otras especies. Nuestros hallazgos sugieren que los tucanes son especies neofílicas que podrían
beneficiarse de tasas de exploración más altas. Por otro lado, las amazonas parecen ser particularmente neofóbi-
cas, posiblemente debido a sus hábitos alimenticios menos oportunistas en comparación con los tucanes y a su
mayor vulnerabilidad a la depredación en comparación con los guacamayos.
Conclusión: Estos resultados señalan que los tucanes serían más proclives a participar en enriquecimientos
ambientales, mientras que un diseño que utilice olores dentro de objetos más naturales o familiares podría ser
más exitoso para las psitácidas.
Palabras clave: psitácidas; ranfástidos; cautiverio; bienestar animal; centro de rescate; Costa Rica.
3
Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075, Vol. 72: e54616, enero-diciembre 2024 (Publicado Ene. 23, 2024)
information, and losing the opportunity to
exploit new food sources or microhabitats.
Moreover, displaying a vigilant state can also
divert energy and time from other vital activi-
ties such as foraging or mating (Crane & Fer-
rari, 2017). Hence, there exists a clear trade-off
between the costs of displaying neophobia
and its benefits (Greenberg, 2003; Greenberg
& Mettke-Hofmann, 2001), and it is expected
that this behavioural trait varies across species
depending on their ecological particularities.
Neophobia may be advantageous in species
that inhabit high risk habitats or that exhibit
a narrow or specialised trophic niche, while it
could be detrimental for more generalist spe-
cies which could benefit from exploring unpre-
dictable and variable new resources (Brown
et al., 2013; Greenberg, 2003; Greenberg &
Mettke-Hofmann, 2001; Mettke-Hofmann et
al., 2002). Furthermore, for species living in
complex habitats, decreased neophobia would
entail higher exploratory rates, providing a sig-
nificant increase in acquired information which
can impact the individual fitness (Biondi et al.,
2010; Greenberg, 2003; Greenberg & Mettke-
Hofmann, 2001; Mettke-Hofmann et al., 2002).
However, it is also true that differences in neo-
phobia can also be found within individuals of
the same species, caused by previous experienc-
es or personality types (Ensminger & Westneat,
2012; Fox & Millam, 2004; Herborn et al., 2010;
Medina-García et al., 2017).
Therefore, the aim of this study was to eval-
uate differences in participation in an olfactory
enrichment depending on the taxonomic group
(Amazons Amazona / Macaws Ara / Toucans
Ramphastos), as well as comparing neopho-
bia response between them. Since neophobia
seems to be particularly pronounced for at least
some psittacine species (Mettke-Hofmann et
al., 2002; Van Horik, 2014; Wilson & Luescher,
2006), we predicted that toucans would show
a higher participation in the enrichment. In
parallel, we also expected that the mean latency
time to interact with the enrichment would be
lower in toucans due to psittacine tendency
towards neophobia.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Study location and animals tested:
Research was conducted in five Costa Rica
wildlife rescue centers: The Toucan Rescue
Ranch, Centro de Rescate Las Pumas, Parque
Zoológico y Jardín Botánico Nacional Simón
Bolívar, Rescue Center Costa Rica, and Res-
cate Wildlife Rescue Center. The study was
performed with 73 red-lored amazons (Ama-
zona autumnalis), 101 yellow-naped amazons
(Amazona auropalliata), 52 scarlet macaws
(Ara macao), 11 great green macaws (Ara
ambiguus), 10 keel-billed toucans (Ramphas-
tos sulfuratus) and 10 yellow-throated toucans
(Ramphastos ambiguus) (Table 1). All indi-
viduals were adults, but sexes were unknown.
Most of the tested birds were non–releasable
animals because they were confiscated pets,
lacking key behaviours to survive in the wild.
Birds were held in communal aviaries, except
for the toucans that were kept individually or
in pairs. Enclosures were provided with natural
vegetation and perches. Environmental enrich-
ment was frequently provided by these centres,
for example wooden toys, wrapped food, for-
aging devices with berries and so forth. The
diet mainly consisted of fruits (banana, apple,
papaya, pineapple, watermelon, cantaloupe),
vegetables (carrot, broccoli, squash, celery, zuc-
chini), beans, corn, and sunflower seeds. Also
boiled eggs, pellets and worms were part of the
diet for the toucans.
All experiments were non-invasive and
based exclusively on behavioural tests in which
animals could decide whether to participate or
not. All applicable international, national, and/
or institutional guidelines for the care and use
of animals were followed. The experiments
were performed in accordance with the ethical
standards of each institution at which the stud-
ies were conducted, with Costa Rica animal
welfare law (Ley No. 7451. Ley del bienestar
de los animales, 1994) and with the Spanish
Government animal research legislation (Real
Decreto 53/2013, 2013).
4Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075 Vol. 72: e54616, enero-diciembre 2024 (Publicado Ene. 23, 2024)
Table 1
Detailed description of the enclosures in each centre and the number of individuals of each species held in each facility.
Center Size of the enclosure
(L × W × H, m)
Ara
macao
Ara
ambiguu
Amazona
autumnalis
Amazona
auropalliata
Ramphastos
sulfuratus
Ramphastos
ambiguus
Toucan Rescue Ranch 10 × 3 × 4 6
Toucan Rescue Ranch 10 × 3 × 4 6
Toucan Rescue Ranch 10 × 4 × 4 10 13
Toucan Rescue Ranch 3 × 2 × 3 2
Toucan Rescue Ranch 5 × 3 × 3 2 3
Toucan Rescue Ranch 2 × 2 × 2 2 2
Toucan Rescue Ranch 2 × 2 × 2 4 2
Toucan Rescue Ranch 10 × 4 × 6 2 2
Toucan Rescue Ranch 10 × 4 × 6 2
Toucan Rescue Ranch 4 × 2 × 4 1
Toucan Rescue Ranch 5 × 3 × 4 1
Toucan Rescue Ranch 4 × 2 × 4 1
Toucan Rescue Ranch 4 × 2 × 4 1
Toucan Rescue Ranch 4 × 2 × 4 1
Toucan Rescue Ranch 4 × 2 × 4 1
Toucan Rescue Ranch 4 × 2 × 4 1
Toucan Rescue Ranch 4 × 2 × 4 1
Toucan Rescue Ranch 4 × 2 × 4 1
Toucan Rescue Ranch 4 × 2 × 4 1
Toucan Rescue Ranch 4 × 2 × 4 1
Toucan Rescue Ranch 4 × 2 × 4 1
Toucan Rescue Ranch Release Site 10 × 4 × 4 11 4
Toucan Rescue Ranch Release Site 10 × 4 × 4 10 4
Toucan Rescue Ranch Release Site 4 × 2 × 4 2
Toucan Rescue Ranch Release Site 10 × 4 × 8 6
Las pumas 18 × 4 × 6 10 30
Las pumas 10 × 8 × 4 3 5 10
Las pumas 8 × 6 × 4 5 15
Las pumas 8 × 6 × 4 3
Simón Bolivar Zoo 4.5 × 3.5 × 2.5 3
Simón Bolivar Zoo 6 × 3 × 2.5 2
Simón Bolivar Zoo 3 × 3 × 2.5 4
Simón Bolivar Zoo 3 × 3 × 2.5 2
Simón Bolivar Zoo 3 × 3 × 2.5 1
“Rescate” Wildlife Rescue Center 8 × 6 × 6 10
“Rescate” Wildlife Rescue Center 8 × 6 × 6 10
“Rescate” Wildlife Rescue Center 6 × 3 × 4 1
“Rescate” Wildlife Rescue Center 6 × 3 × 4 1
“Rescate” Wildlife Rescue Center 8 × 6 × 6 7
Rescue Center Costa Rica 10 × 3 × 3 4 10 16
5
Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075, Vol. 72: e54616, enero-diciembre 2024 (Publicado Ene. 23, 2024)
Experimental design and behavioral data
collection: Animals were exposed to an olfac-
tory enrichment consisting of closed plastic
containers (12 × 12 × 7 cm, all were opaque
yellow) with small holes to allow the odours to
come out following the Hernández et al., (2022)
protocol which confirmed that at least toucans
and macaws can discriminate between odours.
Three different odour choices were simultane-
ously presented to the birds: a) water, b) apple
vinegar, c) banana and papaya juice. Prelimi-
nary analyses showed that bird willingness to
participate in the enrichment and latency times
were not determined by the odour type, so
we analyse them together. We kept the three-
choice scent design because we demonstrated
that it was interesting for the birds. Once they
decide to engage with the enrichment, they
alternate interest in the containers depending
on what’s inside. Therefore, the neophobia/
neophilia tendency analysed in this study was
in response to the novel odour containers
rather than to the odour itself, whereas previ-
ous studies revealed that the odour is the deter-
minant of the total time they spend with each
container (Hernández et al., 2022). Treatments
consisted in eight cotton balls impregnated in
60 ml of one of each. The three plastic contain-
ers were placed in a random order inside the
enclosures separated by 50 cm (Fig. 1). After
installing the enrichment, the researcher left
the enclosure and initiated the video recording
of bird responses for 10 min. To establish com-
parable conditions, data collection was always
performed before bird breakfast (between 6:00-
8:00 h). Each enclosure was tested only once, to
avoid pseudoreplication due to the inability to
discriminate between individuals between vid-
eos (but we could discriminate between indi-
viduals within a video by following each bird
movements throughout the entire video record-
ing). Later, we analysed the video recordings to
Fig. 1. Schematic illustration of the experimental design: Three scents (water, vinegar, papaya and banana juice) were
simultaneously presented to the birds in separate plastic boxes. (Author: modified from Hernández et al., 2022).
6Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075 Vol. 72: e54616, enero-diciembre 2024 (Publicado Ene. 23, 2024)
register how many individuals participated in
the experiment and the latency. We considered
participation in the study as the physical con-
tact of the bird (touching any plastic container
with the beak or foot to explore it) with the
containers and we define latency as the delay
between the exposure to the enrichment and
bird physically interacting with it.
Statistical analysis: To evaluate differ-
ences in bird participation between taxonomic
groups (Amazona / Ara / Ramphastos), we
performed a contingency table analysis using
χ2 and Cramer’s V tests. Bird latency to interact
with the olfactory enrichment was analysed
using a Generalized Linear Mixed effect model
(GLMM), fitting normal distribution and iden-
tity link function. The response variable consid-
ered for the model was bird latency (s) (i.e. the
delay between putting the olfactory enrichment
in the enclosure and bird touching it) and we
included the taxonomic group (Genus) as fixed
factor (Amazona spp. / Ara spp. / Ramphastos
spp.). We also set the individual as random fac-
tor to control for possible differences in animal
personality traits. Preliminary analysis showed
that there were no differences in the participa-
tion and latency of the birds between centers, so
we did not include this variable in the models in
order to have greater statistical power.
Results were considered significant at α <
0.05. Data are represented as mean (s) ± stan-
dard error (SE). The software used to perform
the statistical analysis was SPSS 23.0 for Win-
dows (IBM Corp, 2015).
RESULTS
Bird participation: 257 birds were exposed
to the olfactory enrichment: 175 amazons Ama-
zona spp., 63 macaws Ara spp. and 19 toucans
Ramphastos spp. Of them, only 28 (16.00 %)
amazons Amazona spp., 12 macaws (19.04 %)
Ara spp. and 10 toucans (52.63 %) Ramphas-
tos spp. engaged in physical contact with the
enrichment. Data analyses showed that there
were differences in bird participation depend-
ing on the taxonomic group, with amazons and
macaws being much less inclined to interact
with the olfactory enrichment compared to
toucans (χ2 = 14.686, d.f. = 2, P = 0.001; Cra-
mer’s V = 0.239, P = 0.001, Fig. 2).
Bird latency time to interact with the
enrichment: for birds participating in the
enrichment, according to GLMM, the mean
latency time was higher (73.11 ± 17.88 s) in
amazons, Amazona spp., than for Ara spp.
(16.08 ± 5.60 s) or Ramphastos spp. (15.30 ±
Fig 2. Relationship between the number of individuals of each taxonomic group (Amazons Amazona spp. / Macaws Ara spp.
/ Toucans Ramphastos spp.) exposed to the olfactory enrichment and the number of individuals which participated in it: A.
Absolute numbers and B. Percentages.
7
Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075, Vol. 72: e54616, enero-diciembre 2024 (Publicado Ene. 23, 2024)
6.85 s) (Table 2, Fig. 3). Furthermore, the indi-
vidual identity has influence as random factor
over the variance of latency response of the
birds (Table 2).
Table 2
Results of the GLMM analysing latency time (s) exhibited
by the birds which interacted with the olfactory enrichment
depending on the taxonomic group (Amazons Amazona
spp. / Macaws Ara spp. / Toucans Ramphastos spp.) and the
individual as random factor.
Factors F df P
Corrected model 3.814 47 0.029
Genus 3.814 47 0.029*
Individuala
aRandom factor. (Estimate = 2 657.30, SE = 1 097.14, Z–test
= 2.424, P = 0.015). *Genus Amazona (SE= 26.86; t = 2.152;
P = 0.037).
DISCUSSION
The results of this study showed that bird
participation in the olfactory enrichment was
determined by the taxonomic group. As we
expected, toucans engaged with the smell con-
tainers more frequently than amazons and
macaws. This finding indicates that toucans
are more prone towards neophilia compared
to psittacines. Keel-billed and yellow-throated
toucans have been traditionally considered gen-
eralist frugivores (Remsen et al., 1993), but they
are also opportunistic foragers which consume
invertebrate and vertebrate preys, eggs and even
carrion (Cove et al., 2016; Davlantes & Howe,
2018; Romero et al., 2022; Zúñiga-Ortiz, 2014).
Our results may suggest that keel-billed and
yellow-throated toucans are neophilic species,
since neophobia would be disadvantageous
for opportunistic species, as they would lose
chances to exploit unpredictable and unknown
resources. On the contrary, red-lored amazons,
yellow-naped amazons, scarlet macaws, and
great green macaws, are not omnivorous spe-
cies and only feed on seeds, fruits, flowers,
leaves and bark (Benavidez et al., 2018; Berg
et al., 2007; Vaughan et al., 2006; Volpe et
al., 2022), which may explain their tendency
towards neophobia. The main limitation of
this study was the fact that birds were kept in
different housing conditions and that we could
not control the possible influence of social
competition in the willingness of the birds
and the latency times showed to approach the
enrichment. Amazons and macaws are highly
social species which benefit from being kept in
communal aviaries (Seibert, 2006; van Zeeland
et al., 2009) while toucans are usually held
alone or in pairs because they are territorial,
Fig. 3. Mean latency time (s) exhibited by the birds which interacted with the olfactory enrichment depending on the
taxonomic group (Amazons Amazona spp. / Macaws Ara spp. / Toucans Ramphastos spp.).
8Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075 Vol. 72: e54616, enero-diciembre 2024 (Publicado Ene. 23, 2024)
especially during breeding season (Bor-Mikich
et al., 2008). It is probable that the hierarchy
among parrots could delay the interaction of
lower rank individuals with the scent contain-
ers. Consequently, further research is warrant-
ed to gain insight into how hierarchy influences
participation in this type of enrichment. In
large groups, we recommend including as many
enrichment containers as possible, to guarantee
that all individuals have access to it. In this
regard, further research would be needed, test-
ing animal responses individually to further
verify our results.
Regarding the differences found in mean
latency time, we found the delay to interacting
with the olfactory enrichment was significantly
higher in amazons than in macaws and toucans.
These results suggest that amazons are particu-
larly cautious when it comes to exploring new
objects in comparison to macaws and toucans.
This finding can be also linked to a neophobic
tendency in red-lored amazons and yellow-
naped amazons, explained by their life-history
traits. As we mentioned above, amazons do not
prey on other animals nor opportunistically
consume eggs or carrion as toucans do. More-
over, amazons are much smaller psittacines
than macaws so they may be easier to prey
upon, whereas macaws’ predators mainly affect
the nest (Fraixedas et al., 2014; Schruhl et al.,
2012). This could entail that unknown settings
are riskier for amazons than for macaws, so
they would benefit from a neophobic approach.
Moreover, it is also important to highlight
that there were individual differences in the
latency time. As previously described in the
literature, personality and previous experiences
can affect neophobia responses (Ensminger &
Westneat, 2012; Fox & Millam, 2004; Herborn
et al., 2010; Medina-García et al., 2017). There-
fore, each bird response to the enrichment was
not only determined by the taxonomic group,
but probably also by personality types and
the reason why they came to the rescue cen-
tres. Many birds were confiscated because they
were pets while others were born and raised
in the wild but suffered from injuries or other
health problems. Future studies could focus on
evaluating differences in personality traits and
neophobic behaviour depending on the animal
background history.
We found that participation success was
higher in toucans than in amazons and macaws
possibly because of a lesser neophobic reac-
tion to the containers. Red-lored amazons and
yellow-naped amazons seemed to be particu-
larly neophobic species, showing higher latency
times to interact with the enrichment, prob-
ably due to their life-history attributes. Our
results indicate that the container determined
the neophobic/neophilic response, whereas the
odour inside would determine the time spend
which each container (Hernández et al., 2022).
We also highlight that keel-billed and yellow-
throated toucans are highly participative in
environmental enrichments, so it would be
of relevance to routinely include this kind of
enrichment in captivity welfare plans. In the
case of amazons and macaws, they seem to
be more cautious so it may be more success-
ful to present odours in natural containers or
to apply them on features in their enclosure.
Finally, the next step would be to evaluate if
this kind of enrichment produces differences
in bird conduct (more desirable behaviours
and less stereotypies) and in the physiological
stress response, to properly assess its impact on
animal welfare.
Ethical statement: the authors declare that
they all agree with this publication and made
significant contributions; that there is no con-
flict of interest of any kind; and that we fol-
lowed all pertinent ethical and legal procedures
and requirements. All financial sources are fully
and clearly stated in the acknowledgments sec-
tion. A signed document has been filed in the
journal archives.
ACKOWLEDGMENTS
We wish to thank all the centres that have
kindly allowed us to conduct the experiments
in their facilities: The Toucan Rescue Ranch,
Centro Rescate Las Pumas, Parque Zoológico
y Jardín Botánico Nacional Simón Bolívar,
9
Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075, Vol. 72: e54616, enero-diciembre 2024 (Publicado Ene. 23, 2024)
Rescue Center Costa Rica and Rescate Wildlife
Rescue Center.
REFERENCES
Benavidez, A., Palacio, F. X., Rivera, L. O., Echevarria, A.
L., & Politi, N. (2018). Diet of Neotropical parrots is
independent of phylogeny but correlates with body
size and geographical range. Ibis, 160(4), 742–754.
Berg, K. S., Socola, J., & Angel, R. R. (2007). Great Green
Macaws and the annual cycle of their food plants in
Ecuador. Journal of Field Ornithology, 78(1), 110.
Biondi, L. M., Bó, M. S., & Vassallo, A. I. (2010). Inter-indi-
vidual and age differences in exploration, neophobia
and problem–solving ability in a Neotropical rap-
tor (Milvago chimango). Animal Cognition, 13(5),
701–710.
Bor-Mikich, S., Jennings, J., & Cubas, Z. S. (2008). Order
Piciformes (Toucans, Woodpeckers). In M. Fowler,
& Z. S. Cubas (Eds.), Biology, medicine, and surgery
of South American wild animals (pp. 180–199). John
Wiley Sons.
Brilot, B. O., Asher, L., Feenders, G., & Bateson, M. (2009).
Quantification of abnormal repetitive behaviour in
captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Beha-
vioural Processes, 82(3), 256–264.
Brown, G. E., Ferrari, M. C., Elvidge, C. K., Ramnarine, I., &
Chivers, D. P. (2013). Phenotypically plastic neopho-
bia: a response to variable predation risk. Proceedings
of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280(1756),
20122712.
Clark, F., & King, A. J. (2008). A critical review of zoo–
based olfactory enrichment. Chemical Signals in Ver-
tebrates, 11, 391–398.
Colton, S., & Fraley G. S. (2014) The effects of environ-
mental enrichment devices on feather picking in
commercially housed Pekin ducks. Poultry Science,
93(9), 2143–2150.
Cove, M. V., Fernandez, C. M., Alvarez, M. V., Bird, S.,
Jones, D. W., & Fagan, M. E. (2017). Toucans descend
to the forest floor to consume the eggs of ground-
nesting birds. Food Webs, 10, 2–4.
Crane, A. L., & Ferrari, M. C. (2017). Patterns of predator
neophobia: a meta-analytic review. Proceedings of
the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284(1861),
20170583.
Davlantes, J., & Howe, H. F. (2018). Keel-billed Toucans
(Ramphastos sulfuratus) on the ground in a tropical
forest restoration experiment. The Wilson Journal of
Ornithology, 130(3), 770–773.
de Haas, E. N., & van der Eijk, J. A. (2018). Where in the
serotonergic system does it go wrong? Unravelling the
route by which the serotonergic system affects feather
pecking in chickens. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral
Reviews, 95, 170–188.
Ensminger, A. L., & Westneat, D. F. (2012). Individual
and sex differences in habituation and neophobia
in House Sparrows (Passer domesticus). Ethology,
118(11), 1085–1095.
Fraixedas, S., Fernández–Llamazares, Á., Rico, A., Bach,
A., Borrós, M., Barriocanal, C., & Boada, M. (2014).
Suitability of the South Caribbean Coast of Costa
Rica for reintroduction of the great green macaw Ara
ambiguous. Natural Resources, 5, 653–665.
Fox, R. A., & Millam, J. R. (2004). The effect of early envi-
ronment on neophobia in orange–winged Amazon
parrots (Amazona amazonica). Applied Animal Beha-
viour Science, 89(1–2), 117–129.
Greenberg, R. S. (1990). Ecological plasticity, neophobia,
and resource use in birds. Studies in Avian Biology,
13, 431–437.
Greenberg, R. (2003). The role of neophobia and neophilia
in the development of innovative behaviour of birds.
In S. M. Reader, & K. N. Laland (Eds.), Animal inno-
vation (pp. 175–196). Oxford University Press.
Greenberg, R., & Mettke–Hofmann, C. (2001). Ecological
aspects of neophobia and neophilia in birds. In V.
Nolan, & C. F. Thompson (Eds.), Current ornithology
(pp. 119–178). Springer.
Herborn, K. A., Macleod, R., Miles, W. T., Schofield, A. N.,
Alexander, L., & Arnold, K. E. (2010). Personality
in captivity reflects personality in the wild. Animal
Behaviour, 79(4), 835–843.
Hernández, M. C., Villada, A. M., & Barja, I. (2022). Onto
the sense of smell in macaws, amazons and toucans:
can they use volatile cues of fruits to make foraging
decisions? Integrative Zoology, 18(4), 762–771.
IBM Corp. (2015). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (Ver-
sion 23.0, Software). IBM Corp, Armonk, United
States of America. https://www.ibm.com/products/
spss-statistics
Jenkins, J. R. (2001). Feather picking and self–mutilation in
psittacine birds. Veterinary Clinics of North America:
Exotic Animal Practice, 4(3), 651–667.
Kalmar, I. D., Janssens, G. P., & Moons, C. P. (2010). Gui-
delines and ethical considerations for housing and
management of psittacine birds used in research.
ILAR Journal, 51(4), 409–423.
Kalmar, I. D., Moons, C. P., Meers, L. L., & Janssens, G.
P. (2007). Psittacine birds as laboratory animals:
refinements and assessment of welfare. Journal of the
10 Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075 Vol. 72: e54616, enero-diciembre 2024 (Publicado Ene. 23, 2024)
American Association for Laboratory Animal Science,
46(4), 8-15.
Krasheninnikova, A., Berardi, R., Lind, M. A., O’Neill, L.,
& von Bayern, A. M. (2019). Primate cognition test
battery in parrots. Behaviour, 156(5–8), 721–761.
Ley No. 7451. Ley del bienestar de los animales. (1994).
Asamblea Legislativa de la República de Costa Rica.
http://www.pgrweb.go.cr/scij/Busqueda/Normativa/
Normas/nrm_texto_completo.aspx?param1=NRTC
&nValor1=1&nValor2=84296&nValor3=108689&str
TipM=TC
Lumeij, J. T., & Hommers, C. J. (2008). Foraging ‘enrich-
ment’ as treatment for pterotillomania. Applied Ani-
mal Behaviour Science, 111(1–2), 85–94.
Medina-García, A., Jawor, J. M., & Wright, T. F. (2017).
Cognition, personality, and stress in budgerigars,
Melopsittacus undulatus. Behavioral Ecology, 28(6),
1504–1516.
Mellor, E., Brilot, B., & Collins, S. (2018). Abnormal repeti-
tive behaviours in captive birds: A Tinbergian review.
Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 198, 109–120.
Mellor, E. L., McDonald-Kinkaid, H. K., Mendl, M. T.,
Cuthill, I. C., van Zeeland, Y. R., & Mason, G. J.
(2021). Nature calls: intelligence and natural foraging
style predict poor welfare in captive parrots. Proce-
edings of the Royal Society B, 288(1960), 20211952.
Mettke‐Hofmann, C., Winkler, H., & Leisler, B. (2002). The
significance of ecological factors for exploration and
neophobia in parrots. Ethology, 108(3), 249–272.
Newberry, R. C. (1995). Environmental enrichment: Incre-
asing the biological relevance of captive environ-
ments. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 44(2–4),
229–243.
Nielsen, B. L., Jezierski, T., Bolhuis, J. E., Amo, L., Rosell,
F., Oostindjer, M., Christensen, J. W., McKeegan,
D., Wells, D. L., & Hepper, P. (2015). Olfaction: an
overlooked sensory modality in applied ethology and
animal welfare. Frontiers in Veterinary Science, 2, 69.
OBrien, J. (2006). Effects of conspecific playback recor-
dings on a pair of Toco Toucans. Shape of Enrichment,
15(3), 3–5.
Real Decreto 53/2013. (2013). Boletín Oficial del Estado
Español, 34, 11370–11421.
Remsen Jr, J. V., Hyde, M. A., & Chapman, A. (1993). The
diets of Neotropical trogons, motmots, barbets and
toucans. The Condor, 95(1), 178–192.
Rodriguez–Lopez, R. (2016). Environmental enrichment
for parrot species: Are we squawking up the wrong
tree? Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 180, 1–10.
Romero, A., Cedeño, A. S., Montaño, J., & Layola, G. O.
(2022). More than Froot Loops: Keel-billed toucans
(Ramphastos sulfuratus) consume vertebrate carrion.
Food Webs, 31, e00223.
Samson, J. (1996). Behavioral problems of farmed ostriches
in Canada. The Canadian Veterinary Journal, 37(7),
412.
Schruhl, D., Beus, T., Abel, T. D., Medler, M. J., Arce, A.,
& Mork, K. (2012). Macaw Cam: Exploratory camera
trap techniques for monitoring and conservation of
Scarlet Macaw (Ara macao) Nests. Huxley Spatial
Institute.
Seibert, L. M. (2006). Social behaviour of psittacine birds.
In A. U. Luescher (Ed.), Manual of Parrot Behavior
(pp. 43–48). Blackwell Publishing.
van Hoek, C. S., & Ten-Cate, C. (1998). Abnormal behavior
in caged birds kept as pets. Journal of Applied Animal
Welfare Science, 1(1), 51–64.
Van Horik, J. O. (2014). Comparative cognition and beha-
vioural flexibility in two species of neotropical parrots
(Doctoral dissertation). Queen Mary University of
London, United Kingdom.
van Zeeland, Y. R., Spruit, B. M., Rodenburg, T. B., Rieds-
tra, B., van Hierden, Y. M., Buitenhuis, B., & Lumeij,
J. T. (2009). Feather damaging behaviour in parrots:
A review with consideration of comparative aspects.
Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 121(2), 75–95.
Vaughan, C., Nemeth, N., & Marineros, L. (2006). Scarlet
Macaw, Ara macao, (Psittaciformes: Psittacidae) diet
in Central Pacific Costa Rica. Revista de Biología Tro-
pical, 54(3), 919–926.
Volpe, N. L., Thalinger, B., Vilacoba, E., Braukmann, T. W.,
Di Giacomo, A. S., Berkunsky, I., & Kopuchian, C.
(2022). Diet composition of reintroduced Red-and-
Green Macaws reflects gradual adaptation to life in
the wild. The Condor, 124(1), duab059.
Wells, D. L. (2009). Sensory stimulation as environmental
enrichment for captive animals: A review. Applied
Animal Behaviour Science, 118(1–2), 1–11.
Wilson, L., & Luescher, A. U. (2006). Parrots and fear. In
A. U. Luescher (Ed.), Manual of parrot behavior (pp.
225–231). Blackwell Publishing.
Young, R. J. (2013). Environmental enrichment for captive
animals. John Wiley & Sons.
Zúñiga-Ortiz, A. (2014). Primer reporte de depredación de
Ramphastos ambiguus (Rhamphastidae) sobre ratones
en el Parque Nacional Braulio Carrillo, Costa Rica.
Zeledonia, 18(2), 67–69.