1
Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075, Vol. 73 (S2): e64548, mayo 2025 (Publicado May. 15, 2025)
Differences in foraging strategies and morphology of
five warbler species (Parulidae) in a tropical mangrove forest
Gilbert Barrantes*1,2; https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8402-1930
Ana Catalina Sánchez-Quirós1; https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8714-4327
Luis Sandoval1, 2, 3; https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0793-6747
1. Escuela de Biología, Universidad de Costa Rica, San José, Costa Rica; gilbert.barrantes@gmail.com (*Correspondence);
catasq@gmail.com
2. Centro de Investigaciones en Biodiversidad y Ecología Tropical, Universidad de Costa Rica, San José, Costa Rica.
3. Laboratorio de Ecología Urbana y Comunicación Animal, Escuela de Biología, Universidad de Costa Rica, San José,
Costa Rica; biosandoval@gmail.com
Received 02-IX-2024. Corrected 13-I-2025. Accepted 04-III-2025.
ABSTRACT
Introduction: Understanding the factors underlying the interactions among closely related species continue to be
an important issue in ecology and evolution. Warblers have been the group model for studying niche partitioning
through interspecific competition, without considering other possible factors. A more recent approach includes
species-specific morphologies as an important factor that correlates with habitat (or niche) partitioning.
Objective: to compare the foraging strategies of five warbler species (Setophaga p. erithachorides, S. petechia,
Protonotaria citrea, Parkesia noveboracensis, and Leiothlypis peregrina) in a mangrove forest, and to compare
the foraging strategies of S. p. erithachorides, the only resident warbler, between periods (presence vs. absence of
migrants).
Methods: We collected information on substrates and maneuvers used during foraging for the five warbler spe-
cies during 39 sampling sessions. Each session consisted of recording information for 9 successive days every
two weeks.
Results: P. noveboracensis differed extensively from the other species in the foraging substrates and maneuvers.
The other species formed a more compact group, but some differences in both, substrates and maneuvers, were
detected between S. p. erithachorides and L. peregrina. Differences in forging strategies correlate with differences
in the morphology among the five warbles species. The foraging strategy of S. p. erithachorides differed between
periods. This warbler used the substrates similarly in both periods, but used other maneuvers or change their
frequency when migrants are present.
Conclusion: differences in the foraging strategy of five warblers can be attributed to a process of morphological
and behavioral adaptation to an adaptive scape that includes differences in microhabitat, and biotic interactions
(e.g., predation, prey availability) rather than exclusively to interspecific competition.
Keywords: substrate use; capture maneuvers; warbler morphology; Mangrove warbler; wintering warblers.
RESUMEN
Diferencias en las estrategias de alimentación y morfología de cinco especies de reinitas
(Parulidae) en un bosque de manglares tropicales
Introducción: La comprensión de los factores que subyacen a las interacciones entre especies estrechamente
relacionadas sigue siendo un tema importante en ecología y evolución. Las aves de la familia Parulidae han sido
https://doi.org/10.15517/rev.biol.trop..v73iS2.64548
SUPPLEMENT
SECTION: ECOLOGY
2Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075 Vol. 73 (S1): e64548, mayo 2025 (Publicado May. 15, 2025)
INTRODUCTION
Ecological-interactions studies among
closely related species, particularly within a
particular habitat, have a long history in ecology
(MacArthur, 1958; Miller, 1967; Pianka, 1994).
This has been a central issue to understand
habitat partitioning and competition among
species, species-specific adaptive responses,
and the importance of such interactions in
shaping the evolution within a particular group
of species (MacArthur, 1972; Ricklefs & Miles,
1994; Winkler & Leisler, 1985). For decades,
interaction among closely related species spun
around competition and niche partitioning. A
large number of investigations have focused
on measuring differences among species and
attributing such differences to competition,
without certainly tested if competition was
the source of the differences detected, and
much less, the effect of competition on spe-
cies reproduction (Strong, 1984). Competition
cannot be completely discarded as a potential
factor influencing the coexistence of related
species in a community, since in some groups
of species competition has been important in
shaping the structure of communities (Tilman,
1987; Tilman, 1994). However, in other cases
careful conducted experiments demonstrated
that competition was inexistent, in groups of
species for which competition had previously
been considered an important ecological fac-
tor shaping the communities (Wiens, 1989;
Wise, 1993).
A more recent approach integrates mor-
phology and ecology (competition included as
a potential but not an exclusive factor) under
an evolutionary framework to understand the
habitat partitioning and foraging strategies of a
group of closely related species (Losos, 1990a;
Losos, 1990b; Losos 1990c; Losos & Miles,
1994; Norberg, 1994). This approach opened
the opportunity to consider habitat (and niche)
partitioning as an adaptive response of [indi-
viduals within species] species to variation in
the environment, considering environment as
the interaction between abiotic (e.g., vegeta-
tion structure, ambient temperature) and biotic
factors (e.g., resource availability, predation).
Within birds, new world warbles (Parulidae)
are a group model that has been the focus of
el grupo modelo para estudiar la partición de nichos por medio de competencia interespecífica, sin considerar
otros posibles factores. Un enfoque más reciente incluye las morfologías específicas de las especies como un factor
importante que se correlaciona con la partición del hábitat (o nicho).
Objetivo: comparar las estrategias de alimentación de cinco especies de reinitas (Setophaga p. erithachorides, S.
petechia, Protonotaria citrea, Parkesia noveboracensis y Leiothlypis peregrina) en un bosque de manglares, y com-
parar las estrategias de alimentación de S. p. erithachorides, la única reinita residente, entre períodos (presencia
vs. ausencia de migrantes).
Métodos: Recopilamos información sobre sustratos y maniobras utilizadas durante la alimentación de las cinco
especies de reinitas durante 39 sesiones de muestreo. Cada sesión consistió en registrar información durante 9
días sucesivos cada dos semanas.
Resultados: P. noveboracensis difirió ampliamente de las otras especies en los sustratos y maniobras de alimen-
tación. Las otras especies formaron un grupo más compacto, pero se detectaron algunas diferencias tanto en
los sustratos como en las maniobras entre S. p. erithachorides y L. peregrina. Las diferencias en las estrategias de
alimentación se correlacionan con las diferencias en la morfología entre las cinco especies de reinitas. La estrategia
de alimentación de S. p. erithachorides difirió entre períodos. Esta reinita utilizó los sustratos de manera similar en
ambos períodos, pero utilizó otras maniobras o cambió su frecuencia cuando hay migrantes presentes.
Conclusión: las diferencias en la estrategia de alimentación de las cinco reinitas se pueden atribuir a un proceso
de adaptación morfológica y conductual a un entorno adaptativo que incluye diferencias en el microhábitat y
las interacciones bióticas (por ejemplo, depredación, disponibilidad de presas) en lugar de exclusivamente a la
competencia interespecífica.
Palabras clave: uso de substrato; maniobras de captura; morfología de reinitas; reinita de manglar; reinitas
invernantes.
3
Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075, Vol. 73 (S2): e64548, mayo 2025 (Publicado May. 15, 2025)
attention in competition and niche partitioning
(MacArthur, 1958), and on their interaction
with their tropical counterparts during the win-
tering season (Keast, 1980).
The mangrove warbler (Setophaga pete-
chia erithachorides) is the only warbler pres-
ent during the non-wintering season of North
America migratory birds in Costa Rican man-
groves (Barrantes, 1998). However, during the
overwintering period, the mangrove warbler
shared its habitat with large numbers of other
warblers, including the yellow warbler (S. pete-
chia). Yellow warblers consist of a large group
of subspecies, including the mangrove warbler
(Lowther et al., 1999). This particular condition
permits the comparison of foraging strategies
in a group of related birds inside of a relatively
simple tropical forest, the mangrove forest.
Additionally, the arriving of migratory warblers
permits contrasting the foraging strategies of
the mangrove warbler, and thus its flexibility
(plasticity), between two very different condi-
tions: being the only warbler in its habitat,
and sharing the habitat with a large number of
other family members. We specifically focus on
answering the following questions: (1) Do the
foraging strategies differ among five warbler
species (Setophaga p. erithachorides, S. petechia,
Protonotaria citrea, Parkesia noveboracensis,
and Leiothlypis peregrina) in the mangrove
forest? We particularly focus on two aspects
of foraging strategies: the substrates use by
warblers while foraging, and the maneuvers
they use to attempting capturing prey. In this
case we predict that those species more similar
in morphology shared similar foraging strate-
gies. (2) Do the mangrove warblers modify its
foraging strategy when migratory warblers are
present? We expect that if migratory warblers
with similar foraging strategies are abundant,
the mangrove warbler will modify its forag-
ing strategy. A plastic response that allows
mangrove warblers to reduce competition with
other family members.
MATERIAL AND METHODS
Description of the study area: We con-
ducted this research from July 1986 to Decem-
ber 1987 in a mangrove forest in Chomes,
Puntarenas, Costa Rica (10°, 02’ N; 84°, 54’
W). We delimited an area of 70 ha in which
all data were collected. Climatically, the region
is characterized by a long dry season, from
December to May or June, with the rainy sea-
son that includes the other months (Epifanio
et al., 1983).
The vegetation in the area vary according
to the tides influence. The zone of greater influ-
ence includes trees of Rhizophora mangle, R.
harrisonii, Avicennia germinans, A. bicolor, and
Laguncularia racemosa (Jiménez & Soto, 1985),
but in areas above the tides influence (or nearly
so) plants such as Volkameria pittieri, Conocar-
pus erectus, Prosopis juliflora, Pithecellobium
dulce, Capparis odoratissima and Cynophalla
flexuosa are common.
Substrates and foraging maneuvers: To
record the use that different warbler species
made of the mangrove forest we defined six for-
aging substrates (Supplementary Table 1). The
categorization of substrates was based on pre-
liminary observations of warblers in the study
area. To evaluate if each warbler species used
a different set of maneuvers to capture prey, we
defined 15 different maneuvers (Supplemen-
tary Table 1) based on preliminary observations
of warblers in the study area and published
information by Holmes et al. (1979); Fitzpatrick
(1980); and Robinson & Holmes (1982). These
maneuvers were defined as a combination of
specific maneuvers on a particular substrate
(Supplementary Table 1), following Fitzpatrick
(1980), Holmes et al. (1979), and Remsen &
Robinson (1990).
Collected and preparation of data: To
compare the foraging pattern of each warbler
4Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075 Vol. 73 (S1): e64548, mayo 2025 (Publicado May. 15, 2025)
species we collected data along transects hap-
hazardly selected in the interior and border of
the mangrove. All observations were collected
from 630 to 1 200 h for 9 days every two weeks
for a total of 39 sampling sessions. Prior to col-
lect the information we created a unique letter
code for substrates (e.g., a = trunk, e = foliage)
and a unique numeric code for the maneuvers
(e.g., 1 = gleaning, 2 = probing).
We used a voice recorder (Sony M-570V
Handheld Cassette Voice) to collect the for-
aging activity of warblers. When a warbler
was found foraging, we described to the voice
recorder the sequence of different substrates
and different maneuvers that the warbler used
during the observation time. For instance, a
sequence of a1-e2-a2 means that during the
observation time the bird glean on a trunk,
then probe in the foliage, and finally probe in a
trunk. We then played back the tape to extract
the time that each individual bird spent on each
substrate and used each specific maneuver. For
each individual we summed the effective time
for each substrate and maneuver. We also had
a code to indicate that the bird was inactive.
This allowed us to subtract this time from the
total time recorded for a specific individual and
obtain the effective time.
For each individual of each species for
which we obtained a foraging sequence, we
calculated the weighted mean to compensate
for differences in the total time we observed
each individual. For example, if T1 was the total
effective time of the sequence of bird 1, Si the
time the bird foraged in the substrate i, and ΣT
the sum of the effective time of all birds of the
same species in a particular sampling session,
we calculated the weighted mean as: Si*T1/
ΣT. We next sum the weighted means of all
individuals of each species for each particular
sampling session to have an entrance (row) for
species for sampling session. This approach has
two advantages; first, it compensates for differ-
ences in the effective time across individuals,
making the data comparable across species;
and second, we reduced the effect of pseudo-
replicates, because it was likely that we recorded
some of the same individuals in consecutive
days in a sampling session. We used the same
approach for the time warbles spent in each of
the capturing maneuvers.
Morphological data: We obtained mea-
surements of seven morphological traits 1)
tarsus length [from the intertarsal joint to the
middle of the sole of the foot], 2) wing chord
length [unflattened], 3) tail length, 4) culmen
length, 5) culmen width, 6) culmen depth, and
7) weight, from specimens of the same warbler
species, from the ornithology collection of
Museo de Zoología, CIBET, Universidad de
Costa Rica. We performed a Principal Compo-
nent Analysis on morphological traits to reduce
the number of variables. The first component
explained 81% of the variance corresponding
to the seven morphological traits. Therefore,
we used the first PCA as a proxy of the birds
morphology for comparisons.
Statistical analyses: We used two differ-
ent approaches to compare the use of sub-
strates and capturing maneuvers across species.
First, we compared the variation in time birds
spent foraging in each substrate and using each
capturing maneuver across species with the
coefficient of variation (cv). Lower cv values
indicate that birds used substrates or maneu-
vers more homogeneously.
Second, analyses of habitat use and for-
aging behavior include, in most cases, mul-
tiple variables, making univariate statistical
analyses unpractical. Considering the type of
data obtained in this study, we took a differ-
ent approach. We used statistics designed for
comparing species composition across com-
munities, but instead of species abundances we
fed the models with frequencies, specifically
with the time (number of seconds) an indi-
vidual of a given warbler species forage on
each specific substrate or spend in each of the
foraging maneuvers.
We constructed two matrices, one for sub-
strates and another for maneuvers. The col-
umns of these matrices consisted of either
the substrates or the maneuvers, and the rows
included several entries of each species. Each
5
Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075, Vol. 73 (S2): e64548, mayo 2025 (Publicado May. 15, 2025)
row including the number of seconds birds of
a given species forage on each specific substrate
or spend on each specific maneuver during
each sampling session (see above).
To test for differences in substrates and
maneuvers across species and for Setophaga
p. erithachorides between seasons (presence
vs. absence of migrants), we used the permu-
tational analysis of variance (PEMANOVA)
(vegan::adonis2; Oksanen et al., 2020). We test-
ed for homogeneity of multivariate variances,
using the vegan::betadisper function to test for
equal dispersion in the data, and found homo-
geneous variances (P values ranged from 0.16
to 0.68) in all cases.
The morphology was compared among
warbler species with a linear model. We includ-
ed the PCA scores corresponding to the first
component as the response variable and the
species as the predictor. We then compared
between species the PC1 means with the
functions emmeans and pairs of the package
emmeans (Lenth et al., 2018). We used the R
statistical language for all analyses (R Core
Team, 2024).
RESULTS
General results: We collected informa-
tion on foraging substrates and maneuvers
employed to capture prey for 12 warbler species
in the mangrove forest. However, only 5 species
had enough sample size to be included in the
analyses (Setophaga p. erithachorides, S. pete-
chia, Protonotaria citrea, Parkesia noveboracen-
sis, and Leiothlypis peregrina). The abundance
of migrants fluctuated over the study period
but it increased rapidly after the first arrivals,
overpassing by far the number of S. p. eritha-
chorides (Fig. 1).
Foraging substrates and foraging maneu-
vers- all species: The time warblers foraged
on each substrate varied widely across spe-
cies (Supplementary Table 2), but with spe-
cies aggregated in two groups. The first group
included S. p. erithachorides, S. petechia and
P. citrea which used the substrates more
homogeneously (lower cv values) than the sec-
ond group that included P. noveboracensis and
L. peregrina (Fig. 2A).
The use of capture maneuvers was more
heterogeneous than the use of substrates as
indicated by higher values of the cv (Fig. 2B;
Supplementary Table 3), but there were some
patterns. Two of the species (S. petechia and
L. peregrina) had higher mean cv values, indi-
cating an even more heterogenous used of
maneuvers than S. p. erithachorides and P. citrea;
with P. noveboracensis having a larger varia-
tion in maneuvers used across foraging ses-
sions. Hence, P. noveboracensis and L. peregrina
showed a narrower preference for substrates
than the other warblers, and S. petechia and
L. peregrina showed a narrower preference for
maneuvers (Fig. 2A-B).
The use of substrates (analyzed with PER-
MANOVA) also indicated a marked difference
across species (Table 1A; Fig. 2C). Parkesia
noveboracensis differed notably from the other
warblers that showed a larger similarity in
the use of different substrates. However, even
within this compact group (Setophaga p. eritha-
chorides, S. petechia, Protonotaria citrea, and
Fig. 1. Abundance of the four (S. petechia, P. citrea, P.
noveroracensis, and L. peregrina) migrant warblers (black
dots) and the resident S. p. erithachorides (white dots).
The absence of migrants (0 individuals) is consequence
of migrants flying to their breeding grounds in North
America.
6Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075 Vol. 73 (S1): e64548, mayo 2025 (Publicado May. 15, 2025)
Fig. 2. Comparison of foraging strategies of the five warbler species: Setophaga petechia erithachorides [S.p.e], S. petechia
[S.p], Protonotaria citrea [P.cit], Parkesia noveboracensis [P.nov], and Leiothlypis peregrina [L.pe]. Coefficient of variation
(mean and confidence intervals) calculated for the time warblers used the different foraging substrates (a) and the foraging
maneuvers (b). Use of substrates by the five warbler species (c) and excluding P. noveboracensis (d). Use of foraging
maneuvers by the five warbler species (e), and excluding P. noveboracensis (f). The squares represent the centroids and the
ellipses are the 95% confident intervals.
7
Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075, Vol. 73 (S2): e64548, mayo 2025 (Publicado May. 15, 2025)
L. peregrina) the foraging substrates used by
L. peregrina differed from those used by S. p.
erithachorides (Table 1B; Fig. 2D).
The use of foraging maneuvers showed
a similar pattern than the use of foraging
substrates. P. noveborascensis stand out from
the other four species which group together
(Table 1C; Fig. 2E). When P. noveborascensis
was excluded from the analysis, L. peregrina
differed in the use of foraging maneuvers with
S. p. erithachorides (Fig. 2F; Table 1D). This
indicated that P. noveborascensis used a set of
substrates and foraging maneuvers that notably
differed from the substrates and maneuvers
used by the other four warbler species. On the
contrary, S. p. erithachorides used a wider range
of substrates and maneuvers than the other spe-
cies (Fig. 2C-F).
Foraging substrates and foraging maneu-
vers- S. p. erithachorides: The foraging sub-
strates used by S. p. erithachorides did not differ
whether the migrant warblers were present or
not (F = 1.26, P = 0.313). However, the used
of foraging maneuvers differed between both
periods (F = 3.55, P = 0.004). When migrants
were present, S. p. erithachorides used more
maneuvers, and slightly more homogeneously
as indicated by cv values (cvpresent = 3.48, cvabsent
= 3.60).
Warblers morphology: The morphology
differed between groups of species. S. p. eritha-
chorides, S. petechia, and L. peregrina did not
differ in their morphology (Fig. 3). P. citrea and
P. noveborascensis differed from the two Setoph-
aga and L. peregrina, and P. noveborascensis dif-
fered from the other four species (Fig. 3).
DISCUSION
Behavioral ecology studies often provide
relevant information on the factors underlying
the coexistence of closely related species. In this
study warblers exhibited foraging strategies that
differed remarkably among species in a man-
grove forest. There are two well-defined groups
(Fig. 2C-F); one of such groups includes only
a single species, the ground foraging Parkesia
noveborascensis, which stands out from the
rest of species. This is a species that forages
primarily on the ground, gleaning for insects
Table 1
Use of foraging substrates and foraging maneuvers by five warbler species (Setophaga p. erithachorides, S. petechia,
Protonotaria citrea, Parkesia noveboracensis, and Leiothlypis peregrina) in a mangrove forest.
ASubstrates use: all species
Factor DF Sum of squares R2F P
Species 4 12.31 0.76 80.41 0.001
Residual 102 3.90 0.24
Total 106 16.22 1.00
BSubstrate use: excluding P. noveborascensis
Species 3 1.08 0.27 10.27 0.001
Residual 83 2.92 0.73
Total 86 4.00 1.00
CForaging maneuvers: all species
Species 4 10.65 0.72 66.70 0.001
Residual 103 4.11 0.28
Total 107 14.76 1.00
DForaging maneuvers: excluding P. noveborascensis
Species 3 0.88 0.25 9.15 0.001
Residual 83 2.66 0.75
Total 86 3.54 1.00
8Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075 Vol. 73 (S1): e64548, mayo 2025 (Publicado May. 15, 2025)
and other invertebrates on bare soil or in the
litter. The other warblers form a more compact
group (Supplementary Table 2). They primar-
ily forage by gleaning prey from leaves and
twigs, but there are some differences between
S. p. erithachorides and L. peregrina. Although,
both species forage mostly by gleaning, S. p.
erithachorides glean 45% of its time on leaves
and 46% of its time on twigs, while L. peregrina
glean 80% of its time on leaves, making the
foraging of this species different from the other
species in the group (Fig. 2, Supplementary
Table 3).
Morphology is usually associated with par-
ticular foraging strategies and microhabitat
use in different groups of species (Brandl,
et al., 1994, Fitzpatrick, 1985; Losos, 1990c;
Moermond & Denslow, 1985; Norberg, 1994).
Hence, species with similar morphologies, as
a result of closely phylogenetic relatedness (or
convergent evolution) tend to occupy similar
microhabitats and use similar foraging strate-
gies (Brandl, et al., 1994, Losos, 1990c). Within
the studied warblers, the most distant species
(i.e., P. noveboracensis) has the most discrepant
morphology and foraging strategy (Lovette &
Bermingham, 1999; Klein et al., 2004; Lovette
et al., 2010). Protonotaria citrea and L. per-
egrina (= Vermivora peregrina) are, each one,
placed in a different clade but both closer to
the Setophaga (= Dendroica) clade than P. n o v e -
borascensis (Lovette et al., 2010). Protonotaria
citrea differed morphologically from Setophaga
species, but the morphology of Leiothlypis is
similar to that of Setophaga spp. However, Pro-
tonotaria have a similar foraging strategy than
both Setophaga species, despite their morpho-
logical differences, while the foraging pattern
of L. peregrina differed at least from that of S.
p. erithachorides, despite their high similarity
in morphology. In the case of Setophaga, both
taxa have similar morphology and similar for-
aging strategies. Therefore, although morphol-
ogy associates with similar foraging strategies
in this group of warblers, there are some subtle
differences in foraging patterns likely deter-
mined by species specific foraging adaptations
(e.g., preference for certain prey type) or other
morphological features (e.g., wing loading) not
considered in this study.
The foraging strategy of S. p. erithacho-
rides changes when migrants arrive. The use of
foraging substrates does not change but there
was a tendency to use other foraging maneu-
vers (e.g., gleaning of the ground) or use some
maneuvers more frequently (e.g., hanging, hov-
ering) when migrants were present. Competi-
tion is usually advocated to explain changes in
habitat use and foraging behavior, particularly
when species interact with similar and abun-
dant counterparts with similar morphology and
foraging strategies (Bennett, 1980; Cody 1985;
Diamond, 1978; MacArthur, 1958). However,
other factors may usually explain the changes
in habitat or foraging strategies attributed ini-
tially to interspecific competition (Morse, 1985;
Strong, 1984; Wiens, 1989). For instance, the
period of winter residence coincides with the
period of minimal insect abundance in north-
western Costa Rica (Hespenheide, 1980; Jan-
zen, 1980). This fact could be enough to change
the foraging strategy of S. p. erithachorides dur-
ing the wintering period of migratory warblers,
if birds focus on other (or more) prey types.
Thus, more generalist behavior is expected
Fig. 3. Comparison of the morphology, represented by the
median, lower, and upper quantiles of PC1 scores, of the
five warbler species.
9
Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075, Vol. 73 (S2): e64548, mayo 2025 (Publicado May. 15, 2025)
when resource availability decreases (Carpenter
& MacMillen, 1976; Grant, 1993), but further
evidence is required to support this hypothesis.
Differences and similarities in the foraging
strategies of the five warbles species match the
species’ morphology. This suggests that the for-
aging strategy of each species has been molded
by different (or similar) adaptation processes
along the evolution of each species or group.
Therefore, species that have occupied the same
adaptive landscape or are constrained by their
recent ancestry—such as S. p. erithachorides and
S. petechia (Chavarria-Pizarro et al., 2019)—are
expected to exhibit more similar morphologies
and foraging strategies. Our results provide
little support for the niche partitioning hypoth-
esis, as similar species (morphologically and/
or phylogenetically) use the habitat similarly—
contrary to the prediction that similar species
would use the habitat differently. Competition
cannot be entirely excluded, but it would be
only one aspect, like predation and seasonal
changes in resources, of the adaptive scape in
which each species evolved.
Ethical statement: the authors declare that
they all agree with this publication and made
significant contributions; that there is no con-
flict of interest of any kind; and that we fol-
lowed all pertinent ethical and legal procedures
and requirements. All financial sources are fully
and clearly stated in the acknowledgments sec-
tion. A signed document has been filed in the
journal archives.
See supplementary material
a10v73s2-suppl1
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank José Pablo Marín for measur-
ing the morphology of warbler specimens.
LS thanks the Vicerrectoría de investigación,
Universidad de Costa Rica for the support to
conduct this investigation under project num-
ber C2706.
REFERENCES
Barrantes, G. (1998). Biología y comportamiento de la
reinita de manglar (Dendroica petechia xanthotera).
Brenesia, 49-50, 61–69.
Bennett, S. E. (1980). Interspecific competition and niche
of the American Redstart (Setophaga ruticilla) in
wintering and breeding communities. In A. Keast & E.
S. Morton (Eds.), Migrant birds in the Neotropics (pp.
319–335). Smithsonian Institution Press.
Brandl, R., Kristín, A., & Leisler, B. (1994). Dietary niche
breadth in a local community of passerine birds: An
analysis using phylogenetic contrasts. Oecologia, 98,
109–116.
Carpenter, F. L., & MacMillen, R. E. (1976). Threshold
model of feeding territoriality and test with a
Hawaiian honeycreeper. Science, 194, 639–642.
Chavarria-Pizarro, T., Gomez, J. P., Ungvari-Martin, J., Bay,
R., Miyamoto, M. M., & Kimball, R. (2019). Strong
phenotypic divergence in spite of low genetic struc-
ture in the endemic Mangrove Warbler subspecies
(Setophaga petechia xanthotera) of Costa Rica. Ecology
and Evolution, 9, 13902–13918.
Cody, M. L. (1985). An introduction to habitat selection in
birds. In M. L. Cody (Ed.), Habitat selection in birds
(pp. 4–46). Academic Press.
Diamond, J. M. (1978). Niche shifts and the rediscovery of
interspecific competition: Why did field biologists so
long overlook the widespread evidence for interspeci-
fic competition that had already impressed Darwin?
American Scientist, 66, 322–331.
Epifanio, C. E., Murer, E., & Dittel, A. (1983). Seasonal
changes in nutrients and dissolved oxygen in the Gulf
of Nicoya, a tropical estuary on the Pacific coast of
Central America. Hidrobiologia, 101, 231–238.
Fitzpatrick, J. W. (1980). Foraging behavior of neotropical
tyrant flycatchers. Condor, 82, 43–57.
Fitzpatrick, J. W. (1985). Form, foraging behavior, and
adaptive radiation in the Tyrannidae. Ornithological
Monographs, 36, 447–470.
Grant, J. W. A. (1993). Whether or not to defend? The
influence of resource distribution. Ecology, 74,
171–179.
Hespenheide, H. A. (1980). Bird community structure in
two Panama forests: Residents, migrants, and seaso-
nality during the nonbreeding season. In A. Keast &
E. S. Morton (Eds.), Migrant birds in the Neotropics
(pp. 227–237). Smithsonian Institution Press.
Holmes, R. T., Bonney, R. E., & Pacala, S. W. (1979). Guild
structure of the Hubbard Brook bird community: A
multivariate approach. Ecology, 60, 512–520.
10 Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075 Vol. 73 (S1): e64548, mayo 2025 (Publicado May. 15, 2025)
Janzen, D. H. (1980). Heterogeneity of potential food abun-
dance for tropical small land birds. In A. Keast & E.
S. Morton (Eds.), Migrant birds in the Neotropics (pp.
545–552). Smithsonian Institution Press.
Jiménez, J. A., & Soto, R. (1985). Patrones regionales en la
estructura y composición de los manglares de la costa
pacífica de Costa Rica. Revista Biología Tropical, 33,
25–37.
Keast, A. (1980). Spatial relationship between migratory
parulid warblers and their ecological counterparts
in the neotropics. In A. Keast & E. S. Morton (Eds.),
Migrant birds in the Neotropics (pp. 109–130). Smith-
sonian Institution Press.
Klein, N. K., Burns, K. J., Hackett, S. J., & Griffiths, C. S.
(2004). Molecular phylogenetic relationships among
the wood warblers (Parulidae) and historical biogeo-
graphy in the Caribbean basin. Journal of Caribbean
Ornithology, 17, 3–17.
Lenth, R., Singmann, H., Love, J., Buerkner, P., & Herve,
M. (2018). Emmeans (Version 4.0-3) [Com-
puter software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/
package=emmeans
Losos, J. B. (1990a). Phylogenetic analysis of character
displacement in Caribbean Anolis lizards. Evolution,
44, 558–569.
Losos, J. B. (1990b). The evolution of form and function:
Morphology and locomotion performance in West
Indian Anolis lizards. Evolution, 44, 1189–1203.
Losos, J. B. (1990c). Ecomorphology, performance capa-
bility, and scaling of West Indian Anolis lizards:
An evolutionary analysis. Ecological Monographs, 60,
369–388.
Losos, J. B., & Miles, D. B. (1994). Adaptation, constraint,
and the comparative method: Phylogenetic issues and
methods. In P. C. Wainwright & S. M. Reilly (Eds.),
Morphological ecology: Integrative organismal biology
(pp. 60–98). University of Chicago Press.
Lovette, I. J. & Bermingham, E. (1999). Explosive specia-
tion in the New World Dendroica warblers. Procee-
dings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 266,
1629–1636.
Lovette, I. J. Pérez-Emán, J. L., Sullivan, J. P., Banks, R. C.,
Fiorentino, I., Córdoba-Córdoba, S., Echeverry-Gal-
vis, M., Barker, K. F., Burns, K. J., Klicka, J., Lanyon,
S. M., & Bermingham, E. (2010). A comprehensive
multilocus phylogeny for the wood-warblers and a
revised classification of the Parulidae (Aves). Molecu-
lar Phylogenetics and Evolution, 57, 753–770.
Lowther, P. E., Celada, C., Klein, N. K., Rimmer, C. C., &
Spector, D. A. (1999). Yellow Warbler (Dendroica
petechia). In A. Poole & F. Gill (Eds.), The Birds of
North America (No. 454). Birds of North America,
Philadelphia.
MacArthur, R. H. (1958). Population ecology of some
warblers of northeastern coniferous forests. Ecology,
39, 599–619.
MacArthur, R. H. (1972). Geographical ecology: Patterns in
the distribution of species. Princeton University Press.
Miller, R. S. (1967). Patterns and processes in competition.
Advances in Ecological Research, 4, 1–74.
Moermond, T. C., & Denslow, J. S. (1985). Neotropical
avian frugivores: Patterns of behavior, morphology,
and nutrition, with consequences for fruit selection.
In M. L. Cody (Ed.), Habitat selection in birds (pp.
865–897). Academic Press.
Morse, D. H. (1985). Habitat selection in North American
parulid warblers. In M. L. Cody (Ed.), Habitat selec-
tion in birds (pp. 131–157). Academic Press.
Norberg, U. (1994). Wing design, flight performance, and
habitat use in bats. In P. C. Wainwright & S. M. Reilly
(Eds.), Morphological ecology: Integrative organismal
biology (pp. 205–239). University of Chicago Press.
Oksanen, J., Blanchett, F. G., Friendly, M., Kindt, R., Legen-
dre, P., McGlinn, D. P. R., O’Hara, M. R. B., Simpson,
G. L., Solymos, P., Stevens, M. H. H., Szoecs, E., &
Wagner, H. (2020). Vegan: Community ecology pac-
kage (Version 2.6-6.1) [Computer software]. CRAN.
https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=vegan
Pianka, E. C. (1994). Evolutionary ecology. Harper Collins.
R Core Team. (2024). R: A language and environment
for statistical computing (Version 4.4.0) [Computer
software]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
https://www.R-project.org/.
Remsen, J. V., & Robinson, S. K. (1990). A classification
scheme for foraging behavior or birds in terrestrial
habitats. Studies in Avian Biology, 13, 144–160.
Ricklefs, R. E., & Miles, D. B. (1994). Ecological and evolu-
tionary inferences from morphology: An ecological
perspective. In P. C. Wainwright & S. M. Reilly (Eds.),
Morphological ecology: Integrative organismal biology
(pp. 13–41). University of Chicago Press.
Robinson, S. K., & Holmes, R. T. (1982). Reviewed Fora-
ging behavior of forest birds: the relationships among
search tactics, diet, and habitat structure. Ecology, 63,
1918–1931.
Strong, D. R. (1984). Exorcising the ghost of competition
past: Phytophagous insects. In D. R. Strong, D. Sim-
berloff, L. G. Abele, & A. B. Thistle (Eds.), Ecological
communities: Conceptual issues and the evidence (pp.
28–41). Princeton University Press.
Tilman, D. (1987). The importance of the mechanisms of
interspecific competition. American Naturalist, 129,
769–774.
11
Revista de Biología Tropical, ISSN: 2215-2075, Vol. 73 (S2): e64548, mayo 2025 (Publicado May. 15, 2025)
Tilman, D. (1994). Competition and biodiversity in spatia-
lly structured habitats. Ecology, 75, 2–16.
Wiens, J. A. (1989). The ecology of bird communities: Foun-
dation and patterns. Cambridge University Press.
Winkler, H., & Leisler, B. (1985). Morphological aspects of
habitat selection in birds. In M. L. Cody (Ed.), Habitat
selection in birds (pp. 415-434). Academic Press.
Wise, D. H. (1993). Spiders in ecological webs. Cambridge
University Press.